
Saturday, August 27, 2022
Perception and suggestion

Monday, June 14, 2021
Dream weavers
In Ursula LeGuin's amazing and disturbing novel The Lathe of Heaven, a very ordinary guy finds that he has a completely unordinary ability.
When he dreams, he wakes up and finds that whatever he dreamed has become reality.
George Orr, the protagonist, is terrified by this, as you might imagine. It's not like he can control what he dreams; he isn't able to program himself to dream something pleasant in order to find he has it when he wakes. No, it's more sinister than that. Consider the bizarre, confusing, often frightening content of most of our dreams -- dreams that prompt you to say the next morning, "Where the hell did that come from?"
That is what makes up George's reality.
The worst part is that George is the only one who knows it's happening. When his dream content alters reality, it alters everything -- including the memories -- of the people he knows. When he wakes up and finds that the cityscape has changed and that some people he knew are gone, replaced with others he has never seen before, everyone else's memories changed as well. George wakes to find he has a girlfriend, but she doesn't think it's sudden and weird; being George's girlfriend is what she remembers.
Only George sees that this is just the latest version of a constantly shifting reality.
So when he tries to explain to people what he can do, and (if possible) find a way to stop it, no one believes him. No one... except a ruthless and ambitious psychiatrist who realizes that if he can figure out how to manipulate George's dreams, he can fashion a world to his own desires, using George as a tool to create the reality he wants.
LeGuin's terrifying vision of what happens when grasping amorality meets a naïve but useful skill is turning out not to be far from being realized. George Orr's paranormal ability is the stuff of fiction, of course; but the capacity for influencing our dreams is not.
Nor, apparently, is the potential for using our dreams as a conduit for suggestions that might alter our behavior -- with or without our permission, possibly with or without our knowledge.
I found out yesterday from a friend and loyal reader of Skeptophilia that there is a cohort of powerful corporations who have teamed up to see if there's a way to insert advertising content into our dreams. Xbox, Coors, Microsoft, and Burger King, among others, have been experimenting on volunteers to see if they can introduce targeted advertising while we're asleep, and (especially) while we're at the neurologically hyperactive REM (rapid eye movement) stage of sleep, in order to induce people to alter their behavior -- i.e., purchase the product in question -- once they wake up. They've had some success; a test by Coors found that sixty percent of the volunteers were susceptible to having these kinds of product-based suggestions influence their dream content.
Forty sleep and dream researchers are now pushing back, and have drafted a document calling for regulation of what the corporate researchers are calling "dream incubation." "It is easy to envision a world in which smart speakers—forty million Americans currently have them in their bedrooms—become instruments of passive, unconscious overnight advertising, with or without our permission," the authors state.
My fear is that the profit motive will outweigh any reluctance people might have toward having their dreams infiltrated by corporate content. If cellular service providers were willing to give users a discount on their monthly fees, provided they agreed to allow themselves to be exposed to ads during the nighttime hours, how many people would say, "Sure, okay, go for it"? I know for myself, I'm often willing to tolerate ads on games and video streaming services rather than paying extra to go ad-free. I'd like to think that I'm able to tune out the ads sufficiently that they're not influencing my behavior, but what would happen if I'm exposed to them for all eight to ten hours that I'm sleeping every night?
Not all scientists are concerned about the technique's efficacy, however. "Of course you can play ads to someone as they are sleeping, but as far as having much effect, there is little evidence," said Deirdre Barrett, a dream researcher at Harvard University. "Dream incubation doesn’t seem very cost effective compared with traditional advertising campaigns."
Even if it doesn't have the manipulative capability the corporations are hoping, the idea still scares the hell out of me. When every moment of our days and nights become just another opportunity for monetization, where will it all stop? "I am not overly concerned, just as I am not concerned that people can be hypnotized against their will," said University of Montreal dream researcher Tore Nielsen. "If it does indeed happen and no regulatory actions are taken to prevent it, then I think we will be well on our way to a Big Brother state … [and] whether or not our dreams can be modified would likely be the least of our worries."
Which is it exactly. As I've pointed out before, my main concern is about the increasing control corporate interests have over everything. And here in the United States, the problem is that the people who could potentially pass legislation to limit what the corporations can get away with are being funded largely by corporate donors, so they're not anxious to put the brakes on and see that flow of cash dry up suddenly. It's a catch-22 that would require the government to police its own behavior for no other reason than simple morality and ethics.
And you can guess how successful that is likely to be.
I'm hoping that at least someone is listening, though. I tend to agree with Nielsen; I don't think our dream content will be as easily manipulable, or as behavior-altering, as the corporations hope. But what I'm more worried about is that once we refuse to delineate a hard line around our personal lives, and say to them, "Here, and no further," we've opened ourselves up to there being no part of our personal space that isn't considered a target for monetization.
**********************************
In 1924, a young man named Werner Heisenberg spent some time on a treeless island in the North Sea called Helgoland, getting away from distractions so he could try to put together recently-collected (and bizarre) data from the realm of the very small in a way that made sense.
What he came up with overturned just about everything we thought we understood about how the universe works.
Prior to Heisenberg, and his colleagues Erwin Schrödinger and Niels Bohr, most people saw subatomic phenomena as being scaled-down versions of familiar objects; the nucleus like a little hard lump, electrons like planets orbiting the Sun, light like waves in a pond. Heisenberg found that the reality is far stranger and less intuitive than anyone dreamed, so much so that even Einstein called their theories "spooky action at a distance." But quantum theory has become one of the most intensively tested models science has ever developed, and thus far it has passed every rigorous experiment with flying colors, providing verifiable measurements to a seemingly arbitrary level of precision.
As bizarre as its conclusions seem, the picture of the submicroscopic world the quantum theory gives us appears to be completely accurate.
In Helgoland: Making Sense of the Quantum Revolution, brilliant physicist and writer Carlo Rovelli describes how these discoveries were made -- and in his usual lucid and articulate style, gives us a view of some of the most groundbreaking discoveries ever made. If you're curious about quantum physics but a little put off by the complexity, check out Rovelli's book, which sketches out for the layperson the weird and counterintuitive framework that Heisenberg and others discovered. It's delightfully mind-blowing.
[Note: if you purchase this book using the image/link below, part of the proceeds goes to support Skeptophilia!]

Thursday, April 15, 2021
The mental delete key
About ten years ago, some students in my AP Biology class decided to do an experiment on false memory as their final project.
The setup was simple and elegant. One of the students sat behind a small card table on which there were two dozen objects of various types and sizes, initially covered by a cloth. The test subject came in, sat down, and was told (s)he would be given a memory test at the end of three minutes' time to study the objects on the table. The cloth was removed, the timer started.
At a minute and a half in, the other student running the study -- who until then had been offstage -- came in, picked up one of the objects, and walked off with it. Naturally enough, the test subjects focused on which object she'd picked up.
When the three minutes were up, the test subject was read aloud a series of twelve questions about the experience. The answers to only three of them mattered -- the first one and the last two:
- Question 1: What object did the girl in the blue shirt remove from the table?
- Question 11: The girl who came in and removed an object -- what color was her shirt?
- Question 12: How do you know what color her shirt was?
You've probably already guessed that her shirt wasn't blue; in fact, it was brilliant red. But 95% of the fairly sizable number of test subjects answered "blue." Only two test subjects said "red;" several of them said "I don't remember."
But where it got seriously interesting was how the subjects who said "blue" answered question #12. Because the vast majority of them said, "I remember seeing it." Once again, there were only a couple of outliers who said "Because you told me it was blue in question #1," and one or two who said, "I'm not sure." The remainder were convinced they remembered seeing it. When informed that the other member of the scientific team had been wearing a red shirt, several people flat-out didn't believe it and asked that she come back and prove it to them. One of them even accused her of having changed her shirt!
This has always been one of my favorite examples of how plastic and unreliable human memory is. "I know it happened that way, I remember it" is remarkably thin ice. My students' clever experiment is innocuous enough, but think of the role false memories could play in a court of law -- where someone's freedom, perhaps their life, depends on the people on the witness stand remembering what actually happened.
So that's kind of sobering. But a study this week which appeared in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences gives us some encouraging news; false memories are easier to eradicate than real memories -- indicating they may be stored differently in the brain, and don't get the same weight as memories of events that we really witnessed.
In "Rich False Memories of Autobiographical Events Can Be Reversed," Aileen Oeberst (University of Hagen), Merle Madita Wachendörfer (Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien), Roland Imhoff (Johannes Gutenberg Universität Mainz), and Hartmut Blank (University of Portsmouth) show us a simple protocol by which people can be induced to purge their brains of false or implanted memories. They took a test group of 52 individuals whose parents had also agreed to participate. Without being told what exactly was going on, the test subjects were given a period during which their parents recalled with them a series of childhood memories -- but in each case, two of the "memories" were false and the rest were not. (Examples of false memories implanted were incidents like running away from home, getting lost, or being in a car accident -- more serious and emotion-laden than what shirt someone was wearing!)
As with previous experiments, the test subjects afterward were unable to tell apart the real memories from the false ones; both seemed to exist in their minds with equal intensity. Then the researchers tried two approaches to eradicate the false memories: (1) alerting the test subjects to the possibility that their memories were false, and were due to other sources, such as family narratives; and (2) asking test subjects to describe how they know their memories were true (a little like the "How did you know her shirt was blue?" question my students asked).
Both of them worked, and more interesting still, memories of real events were unaffected when the researchers tried the same strategy on them. Put differently, asking people to slow down and consider their brain's fallibility, and the sources of what they think they recall, had the effect of deleting false memories but not real ones.
Even more interesting was how persistent the effect was. A one-year followup on the test subjects found that the implanted memories were virtually all gone -- asked whether an unreal event had happened in their childhood, almost all the volunteers rejected it.
"By raising participants' awareness of the possibility of false memories, urging them to critically reflect on their recollections and strengthening their trust in their own perspective, we were able to significantly reduce their false memories. Moreover, and importantly, this did not affect their ability to remember true events," said study senior author Hartmut Blank, in an interview with Science Daily. "We designed our techniques so that they can principally be applied in real-world situations. By empowering people to stay closer to their own truth, rather than rely on other sources, we showed we could help them realize what might be false or misremembered -- something that could be very beneficial in forensic settings."