As an avid birdwatcher, I've learned many of the vocalizations of our local species. Some, especially the migratory species we only hear from May to September, I have to relearn every year, but a few of them are so distinct that my ears perk up whenever I hear them. One of my favorites is the whirling, ethereal song of the Veery (Catharus fuscescens):
Bird songs serve two main purposes. They're territorial defense signals and mate attractants. (Which led a former student of mine to say, in some astonishment, "So birds only sing when they're mad or horny?") Songs are usually only done by males, and mostly during the breeding season. Calls, on the other hand, are done by both males and females, at any time of the year, and can mean a variety of things from "there's food over here" to "watch out for the cat" to "hey, howsyamommaandem?" (The latter mostly from birds in the southeastern United States.) Those of you in the eastern half of North America certainly already have heard the difference; our local Black-capped Chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) has a call, the familiar "chicka-dee-dee-dee-dee" that gives the species its name, and a song -- a two-note whistle with the second note a whole step below the first. Listening to them, you'd never guess it was the same bird.
There's an interesting distinction in how animals vocalize. Some vocalizations seem to be innate and hard-wired; the barking of dogs, for example, doesn't need to be learned. A great many bird species, however, including songbirds and parrots, learn vocalizations, and deprived of examples to learn from, never sing. (This includes the amazing mimicry of birds like the Australian Superb Lyrebird (Menura novaehollandiae), which can learn to imitate not only birdsongs but a huge variety of other sounds as well):
The Rifleman is not a songbird, and (if the preceding distinction holds) should be unable to learn vocalizations; any sounds it makes should be instinctive and fixed, like the clucking of a chicken. But the study found that there were variations in the vocalizations of different individuals, and those variations were independent of how closely related they were; what mattered was how nearby they lived to each other, implying that the alterations in sound were learned, not innate.
"The vocal behavior that we were unravelling in this study is very similar to what is known as vocal accommodation in human linguistics," said Ines Moran, of the University of Auckland, who led the research. "It's similar to our ability to adjust our ways of speaking in different social, dialectal, or hierarchical settings -- modulating our voices to better fit in certain social groups."****************************************
No comments:
Post a Comment